Prog. Neuro-Peychopharmao. Vol. 1, pp. 235-247, 1977. Pergamon Press. Printed in Great Britain.

NOOTROPIC DRUGS

C. GIURGEA and M. SALAMA Neuropharmacological Research Department, UCB-Dipha, and Clinical Development, UCB-Dipha, Brussels, Belgium; University of Louvain, Belgium

(Final form, July 1977)

Abstract

1.    Nootropic drugs were proposed as a class of psychoactive drugs that selectively improve efficiency of higher telencephalic integrative activities.

2.    The main features of the nootropic profile consist of: (a) enhancement of learning acquisition; (b) resistance to impairing agents; (c) facilitation of interhemispheric transfer of information; (d) enhanced resistance to brain “aggressions”; (e) increased tonic, cor-tico-subcortical “control”; and (f) absence of usual pharmacological effects of neuro-psychotrnpic drugs.

3.    The animal experimental results are thoroughly corroborated by the data obtained in clinical pharmacology and pharmacotherapeutics supporting the concept that nootropic activity is based on a functional telencephalic selectivity.

4.    The basic mechanism of action at molecular and cellular levels of nootropic drugs is not yet known. Some recent data emphasized a possible role for cerebral ATP.

Keywords: psychotropic drugs, nootropics, piracetam, integrative telencephalic mechanism,

learning, brain injury, hypoxia, interhemispheric transfer.

1. Introduction

The term “nootropic” (noos = mind; tropein = towards) was proposed by us (Giurgea, 1972,

1973) to designate psychotropic drugs that characteristically interfere with the higher telencephalic integrative activity by a direct and selective action.

The main features we presently consider important in defining a nootropic drug are: (a) the enhancement, at least under some conditions, of learning acquisitions as well as the resistance of learned behaviours to agents that tend to impair them; (b) the facilitation of interhemispheric flow of information; (c) the partial enhancement of the general resistance of the brain and particularly its resistance to physical and chemical injuries; (d) the increase in the efficacy of the tonic cortico-subcortical control mechanisms; and (e) the display of above mentioned activities by selective functional impact on higher integrative, telencephalic mechanisms, i.e. partial lack of usual psychological and general pharmacological activities.

Piracetam [ (oxo-2-pyrrolidinyl-1) 2 acetamide] seems to, up to now, correspond best to these definitions as far as animal pharmacology, clinical pharmacology and therapeutics are concerned and will be used as prototype for nootropic drugs.

Ip this review we shall present various features of the nootropic profile under three main headings; animal pharmacology, clinical pharmacology and pharmacotherepeutics. Some considerations to biochemical data and the classification of nootropic drugs will be given in the later part of the paper.

2. Nootropic Profile

2.1.    Enhancement of learning acquisition and resistance to impairing agents

2.1.1.    Animal pharmacology

The various data regarding the animal pharmacology are summarized in Table 1 as well as in Figs. 1 and 2. Recent published data on nootropic drugs are presented in Table 1. One additional very recent contribution of Danielle Lefevre of our laboratory* is given here as an example of piracetam’s beneficial effect in early sensory deprived rats (Giurgea 1977).

Nootropic effect on learning.

Experimental data and references in animal pharmacology

Table 1

Modality

Test/Agent

Species

Reference

water-maze

rat

Giurgea and Mouravieff-Lesuisse

Facilitated

1972

“Y” -maze

rat

Wolthuis, 1971

acquisition

“drinking” test

rat

Wolthuis, 1971

spinal fixation

rat

Giurgea and Mouravieff-Lesuisse,

1971

active

avoidance

goldfish

Bryant et at., 1973

hypoxia

rat

Giurgea et at., 1971 Sara and Lefevre, 1972

Enhanced

Gouret and Raynaud,

resistance

1976

to learning

ECS

rat

Giurgea, 1972

impairment

Sara and David

Remade, 1974 Matthies and Ott

1975

age

rat

Giurgea and Mouravieff-Lesuisse,

1972

chemicals (8-AZA)

rat

ibid., 1971, 1972

sensory

Myslivicek and

deprivation

rat

Hassmanova, 1973 Myslivicek, 1975 Lefevre, 1975 (in Giurgea, 1976a)

Figure 1 gives the general schedule of the study on passive avoidance performance (PAP) in differential environment.

Figure 2 illustrates that rats reared in isolation show impaired retention of PAP as seen by its time decay, and that piracetam readily improves learning performances when injected before the last retention test.

Fig. 1. Effect of rearing in differential environment in rats as regards passive avoidance performance (experimental schedule).

Litter-mates Wistar rats are kept at weaning for 6 weeks in sensory “impoverished” (isol.) or “enriched” (enr.) environment. Both groups are kept in isolation for 24 hr and then submitted to passive avoidance one-trial learning. They are tested for retention 24 hr later (test I) and 96 hr after test I (test II). Thirty minutes before test II (arrow), rat received i.p. saline or piracetam (120 mg/kg).

Fig. 2. Effect of piracetam on the deterioration of passive avoidance performance (PAP) in isolated-reared rats.

From left to right: isolated, non-rejected rats show good avoidance in test I but performance in test II is almost, absent; saline-injected rats achieved very poor avoidance score in test II; in the piracetam-group, performance in test II, though very well maintained, was statistically not different from that in test I.

For comparison (extreme right), a typical experiment in “enriched” non-injected rats illustrating excellent maintenance of avoidance performances from test I to test II is shown.

2.1.2. Clinical pharmacology

Dimond and Brouwers (1976) studied the effect of piracetam (4.8 g/day) in a double blind versus placebo investigation, on sixteen healthy student volunteers. After a 2 weeks’ administration (i.e. the second test session under drug), the drug treated group showed an important and significant improvement as compared to the control group both of direct or delayed recall of verbal learning.

Recent confirmation of those conclusions is provided by Wedl and Suchenwirth (1977) who have replicated a significant improvement of mental performance in a group of seventeen young healthy volunteers (3.2 g/day piracetam for 5 days).

Mindus et al. (1976) studied eighteen middle-aged subjects, mentally healthy, but all of them complaining of some memory decline. Piracetam (4.8 g/day) was given for 4 weeks, versus placebo, in a double blind design. By means of a battery of perceptual and psychomotor tests (such as Critical Flicker Fusion (CFF), Krakau Visual Acuity Test (KVAT) and three paper and

pencil tests), the authors concluded that mental performance in these almost normal middle-aged subjects was significantly improved by the drug.

2.1.3. Therapeutic data

The therapeutic indications reporting an improvement of mental performances and vigilance are numerous and diverse.

Chroma brain syndrome due to ageing

Stegink (1972), in a double blind study involving 196 elderly patients, has compared piracetam with a placebo in the treatment of psycho-organic symptoms occurring in the course of senile involution. Piracetam was administered in a dose of 800 mg, t.i.d., for 8 weeks.

The overall mental condition of the patients treated with piracetam showed a significant improvement as compared to that of the placebo group; statistically significant differences were found for such parameters as disorders of alertness, asthenia, and psychomotor agitation.

Feruglio et at. (1974), Delwaide et ai. (1975), Guilmot and Van Ex (1975), Eckmann (1976) in double blind controlled investigations versus placebo, confirmed these results. Heinitz (1975), Plauchu et al. (1974), Voelkel (1975) and Baving (1975) have also published similar results.

Chronie alcoholism

In acute alcohol withdrawal, Knott and Beard (1973),2 Petty (1973),2 Weckroth (1975) and Week-roth and Mikkonen (1972), have demonstrated in controlled double blind studies the advantage of piracetam over placebo. The piracetam-treated patients experienced at a significantly earlier stage an improvement of their state of alertness and mental performances.

Binder (1974), in a controlled study versus placebo, came to the same conclusion in chronic alcoholics who had been hospitalised for long-terms.

2.2. Facilitation of interhemispheric transfer of information

2.2.1. Animal pharmacology

In curarised rats, piracetam selectively facilitates transcallosal evoked potentials on electrical stimulation of the associative, suprasylvian cortex (Giurgea and Moyersoons, 1972, 1974).

Buresova and Bures (1973, 1976) found that in rats interhemispheric transfer of lateralised visual engrams, acquired during functional hemidecortication, was facilitated by piracetam which also facilitated formation of a secondary engram during monocular learning without hemidecortication.

Figure 3 illustrates one aspect of piracetam’s facilitating aspect of the writing-in callosal interhemispheric mechanism.

2.2.2, Clinical pharmacology

Dimond (1975) tried and succeeded in obtaining human data that are in good agreement with the previously reported animal findings. Indeed, in healthy young volunteers submitted to di-chotic listening of verbal messages, it was found:

(a)    that verbal capacity was significantly improved; and

(b)    that this was due mainly to increased response to information presented to the left ear.

Fig. 3. Effect of piracetam on monocular pattern discrimination learning (L) and on strength of resulting primary and secondary engrams revealed by hemidecorticated re-leaming (R). Brain schemes indicate conditions of experiment on days 1-3 (occluded eye and depressed hemisphere black).

P: rats treated with piracetam; S: rats treated with saline. Ordinate: average number of trials to criterion.

Vertical traces denote SEM-values. (Courtesy of Buresova and Bures, 1976).

The brain, concludes Dimond, seems to be “super-connected” by the drug.

2.2.3. Therapeutic data

The improved interhemispheric transfer and the subsequent improved “cortical integration”, as seen in the Dimond study (1975), may be particularly important for the understanding of the enhancement of the vigilance as well as of the mental performances that were discussed previously.

2.3. Enhanced brain resistance to physical and chemical injuries

Table 2

Nootropic effect and its relationship to brain resistance. Experimental data and references in animal pharmacology

Agent

Modality

References

Enhanced cerebral resistance and mainly speeding-up of brain recovery (Criterium: EEG; species: rabbit)

Giurgea et al., 1970

Hypoxia

Enhanced resistance to sub-cellular lesions (Criterium: electronic microscopy and histochemistry; species: rat)

Schiller, 1974

Enhanced resistance to experimental high-altitude

(Criterium: convulsions; species: rat)

Quadbeck, 1970 (pers.comm.)

Chemicals

(barbiturates,

phenothiazines,

cyanides

EEG protection and enhanced survival percentage (Species: cat, rat)

Moyersoons and Giurgea, 1974;

Hoyer, 1975 (pers.comm.)

Figure 4 shows the “curare-like hypoxia” model developed with J. Dauby, using a short-acting curare-like agent, the oxydipentonium.

In Table 3 are presented the results obtained with piracetam and some other neuro-psychotro-pic drugs. It can be seen that dexamphetamine, pyritinol and sulpiride are inactive (Giurgea, 1977).

Fig. 4. Experimental schedule of the “curare-like hypoxia” model.

Mice are given i.p. 3 mg/kg of oxydipentonium chloride, a short-acting curare-like agent.

It is assumed that an active drug (i.p. or p.o.), provided it is not a simple decurarizing agent, enhances the number of survivors because it confers a CNS protection during the short-lasting asphyxia induced by the curare-like agent.

Effect of piracetam and some other neuro-psychotropic drugs on “curare-like hypoxia” model

Table 3

Dose

mg/kg

i.p.

Survival

Drugs

placebo

drug

Activity*

Centrophenoxine

294

2/10

7/10

+

94

0/10

7/10

+

29

2/10

8/10

+

Dexamphetamine

55

2/10

0/10

0

5.5

2/10

2/10

0

0.55

1/10

3/10

0

Piracetam

800

1/10

10/10

+

454

0/10

9/10

+

142

2/10

6/10

0

Pyritinol

459

1/10

3/10

0

46

1/10

2/10

0

Sulpiride

109

3/10

3/10

0

34

1/10

3/10

0_

11

3/10

2/10

0

* Activity is established by the ratio between the number of survivors in drug treated groups and in the placebo group; + means p <0.05 (Fisher Yates).

2.3.2. Clinical pharmacology

Lagergren and Levander (1974), Cronholm et al. (1975) studied the effect of piracetam in twelve patients who had a heart pace-maker and in whom they were able to induce a moderate, brief and reversible bradycardia. Piracetam was given p. o., (4.8 g/day) for 9 days, in a cross-over double blind study using a placebo.

Among several perceptual and psychomotor tests that responded positively to piracetam, the results of the Critical Flicker Fusion tests can be taken to imply that piracetam counteracts the impairment in performance associated with cerebral hypoxia caused by lowering of heart rate. Isaksson et al. (1975, 1976) analysed the same patients and found by quantitative EEG studies that piracetam, both in normal and bradycardic situations, enhanced the alpha-power spectrum, thus tending to normalise the EEG.

2.3.3. Therapeutic data

The effect of piracetam on cerebral distress consecutive to brain injury, or due to a hypoxic or toxic state, was also investigated.

Calliauw and Marchau (1975) in a double blind controlled trial demonstrated that, in comparison with placebo, piracetam significantly improved state of consciousness in deeply comatous hospitalised patients following head injuries.

C. Giurgea and M. Salama

242

Roquefeuil et al. (1975) measuring some biochemical parameters, showed the improvement of the brain metabolism. Schulte-am-Esch and Pfeifer (1974) and Ciocatto and Bava (1974) have published similar clinical data.

2.4. Increased cortico-subcortical “control”

2.4.1. Animal pharmacology

Piracetam shows a clear effect in two experimental models that require integration at CNS subcortical levels.

As seen in Table 4, piracetam inhibits central nystagmus, i.e. the nystagmus obtained in the awake rabbit by electric stimulation of a diencephalic structure (lateral geniculate body).

On the other hand, piracetam facilitates acquisition of an experience at spinal level probably because it shortens the spinal fixation time (Table 4).

Table 4

Effect of piracetam on the cortico-subcortical “control”

Piracetam: Enhanced tonic

“control”

Modality

Species

Reference

Central (LGB) nystagmus: inhibited

rabbit

Giurgea et al., 1967

Spinal fixation:

rat

Giurgea and Mouravieff

facilitated

Lesuisse, 1971

N.B. Cortical spreading depression (Giurgea, 1972) facilitates central nystagmus and inhibits spinal fixation.

It is important to understand in these two particular preparations the cortico-subcortical inter-relationships, especially the descending ones. As compared to the ascending, cortico-petal relationship, the cortico-fugal neurophysiological tonic regulating influences are widely considered more important (Narikashvili, 1970).

To approach this problem we have used the functional decortication produced by local, cortical KC1-application (Bures et at., 1974). Cortical spreading depression (CSD) as seen in Table 4 facilitates central of nystagmus and inhibits spinal fixation. One can argue therefore that the functional, tonic cortical “control” is inhibiting towards LGB and facilitating towards the particular spinal “memory” consolidation.

Thus the activity of piracetam is equivalent to an enhancement of the cortical tonic control since it inhibits central nystagmus and facilitates spinal fixation.

2.4.2. Clinical pharmacology

In 1967, Oosterveld (see Giurgea et at., 1967) has demonstrated in healthy volunteers that piracetam significantly inhibits the so-called “torsion swing” nystagmus. Later on, Boniver (1974) showed that a caloric nystagmus was also inhibited by piracetam in normal subjects.

2.4.3. Therapeutic data

The remote effects of head injuries have also been studied, and the significant superiority of piracetam as compared to a placebo has been evidenced, e.g. in the treatment of post-traumatic vertigo; this activity may be correlated with the one found for nystagmus.

Aantaa and Meurman (1975), Hakkarainen (1976, pers. comm.), and Ferrey and Bouttier (1972) in controlled double blind studies, have shown that piracetam has a significantly superior

Nootropic drugs

243

effect as compared to placebo in patients with post-concussional syndromes, consisting of symptoms vertigo and headaches.

2.5. Lack of side effects

2.5.1. Animal pharmacology

Table 5

Piracetam: Lack of usual pharmacological activity*

Behavioral

Sedation

Giurgea et at., 1967

Stimulation

Wolthuis, 1971

Locomotion in general

Overton, 1974 (pers. comm.)

Toxicity, etc.

Electro-

physiological

EEG (cortical, subcortical)

Limbic

excitability

Stumpff, 1975 (pers.comm.) Giurgea, 1972-76 Giurgea, 1972-76

Autonomic

Cardiovascular

system

Giurgea, 1972-76

Respiratory

system

Gastrointestinal system

 

Piracetam, while active in previously described situations, is devoid of usual “routine” pharmacological activities even in high doses (Table 5). 3

C. Giurgea and M. Salama

244

(c) Piracetam readily passes such physiological barriers as the blood-brain barrier and the placental barrier. From the biochemical pharmacology data, an interesting EEG-biochemical correlation is noted. Indeed, as we have described above, piracetam significantly enhanced EEG recovery after nitrogen hypoxia in the rabbit. Using similar experimental, conditions, it was observed that in the saline-treated rats, it takes about 2 hr to recover normal neurochemical parameters, whereas in piracetam-treated rats there is a highly–significant speedup recovery. Electronic microscopy studies have also shown that hypoxia-induced polysomial damage in the rat brain and a few other organs was prevented in piracetam-treated animals (Schiller 1974).

As to the intimate mechanisms responsible for the functional telencephalic neuro-pharmacolo-gical selectivity of piracetam, we do not have as yet, as mentioned before, comprehensive, causal interpretation in molecular terms of its therapeutic efficacy.

Recently, however, Nicholson and Wolthuis (1976a, b) claimed that piracetam rather selectively activates brain adenylatekinase (an enzyme facilitating ATP formation in anaerobic conditions) and inhibits cortical release of proline (a putative inhibitory neuro-transmitter). Should those findings be enlarged, and confirmed, they might provide a starting point for a deeper understanding of the way in which nootropic drugs act.

4. Problems of Classification

It seems obvious that, if one takes into account the WHO classification of psychotropic drugs (Shepherd, 1972), nootropic drugs should be considered as a new class independent of neuroleptics, anxiolytic sedatives, antidepressants, psychostimulants or psychodysleptics.

If, however, one keeps to the Delay-Deniker classification, nootropic drugs should be looked upon (as suggested by Boissier, pers. comm.) as a distinct class among the psycho-analeptics which, by definition, includes all drugs that somehow enhance mental efficiency.

Fig. 5. The place of nootropic drugs in the classification of psychiatric drug.

 

In the light of previous considerations, the classification of psychotropic drugs is presented in Fig. 5.

Acknowledgements

The authors are indebted to J. Polderman, M.D. for his assistance in the updating of the bibliography.

The competent secretarial work of Mrs. S. Tourtois is also acknowledged.

Inquiries and reprint requests should be addressed to:

Prof. Dr. C. Giurgea, Head of the Neuropharmacological Research Department.

UCB-Dipha,

Brussels, Belgium.

Nootropic drugs

245

References

AANTAA, E. and MEURMAN, I.H. (1975). The effects of piracetam (Nootropil – ucb 6215) upon the late symptoms of patients with head injuries. J. Intern. Med. Res. J3: 352-355.

BAVING, G. (1975). Untersuchungen liber Piracetam bei stationSren Patienten mit cerebro-vascu-lHr bedingten geistiger Leistungsminderung schwerer obstruktiver Emphisembronchitis und ar-terieller Hypoxie. Therapieuoche. 25: 2421-2427.

BINDER, S. (1974). Die Wirkung des Nootropikums Piracetam auf die kortikale Leistungsfahig-keit chronische Alkoholiker. Munch. Med. Wschr. 116: 2127-2130.

BONIVER, R. (1974). Influence du piracetam sur le fonctionnement du syst&me vestibulaire. Quelques reflexions sur 1’interference mddicamenteuse au niveau du syst&me vestibulaire avec les mdcanismes responsables du nystagmus. Acta O.R.L. Belg. 28: 293-299.

BRYANT, R.C., PETTY, F. and BYRNE, W.L. (1973). Effects of piracetam (SKF 38462) on acquisition, retention and activity in the goldfish. Peychopharmac. (Berl.) 29: 121-130.

BURES, J., BURESOVA, 0. and KRIVANEK, J. (1974). The mechanism and applications of Le&o’s spreading depression of electroencephalographic activity. Academia Prague, pp. 140.

BURESOVA, 0. and BURES, J. (1973). Mechanisms of interhemispheric transfer of visual information in rat. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 33: 673-688.

BURESOVA, 0. and BURES, J. (1976). Piracetam-induced facilitation of interhemispheric transfer of visual information in rat. Paychopharmac. (Bert.) 46: 93-102.

CALLIAUW, L. and MARCHAU, M. (1975). Clinical trials of piracetam in disorders of consciousness due to head injury. Acta Anaeath. Belg. 26j 51-60.

CI0CATT0, E. and BAVA, G.L. (1974). UtilitA dell’impiego di un farmaco nootropo (Piracetam) nella rianimazione degli stati di coma. Clin. Terapeutica.

CR0NH0LM, B., SCHALLING, D. and LEVANDER, S. (1975). On models and measures of alertness and noetic functions. 3rd Congress of the Intern. College of Psychosomatic Medicine, Rome.

DELWAIDE, P.J., YLIEFF, M., GYSELYNCK-MAMBOURG, A.M., DIJEUX, L. and HURLET, A. (1975). Ef-fets du piracetam sur la ddmence sdnile. M6d. Hyg. 33: 1150-1158.

DIM0ND, S.J. (1975). The effects of a nootropic substance on the capacity for verbal memory and learning in normal man. 3rd Congress of the Intern. College of Psychosomatic Medicine, Rome.

DIMOND, S.J. and BROUWERS, E.Y.M. (1976). Increase in the power of human memory in normal man through the use of drugs. Paychopharmac. (Berl.) 49: 307-309.

ECKMANN, F. (1976). Klinische Untersuchungen mit Piracetam. Munch, med. Wachr. 118: 957-958.

FERREY, G. and B0UTTIER (1972). Controlled study of 50 cases of post-traumatic subjective headache syndromes. CongrSs des PsychiAtres et Neurologistes de langue franjaise, LXXAme Stance, Tunis.

FERUGLI0, F.S., MACCHIONE, C. and MOLASCHI, M. (1974). II 2-pirrolidone-acetamide (Piracetam) nella sindrome psico-organica senile. Clin. Terapeutica 51-52.

GIURGEA, C. (1972). Vers une pharmacologie de 1’activity integrative du cerveau. Tentative du concept nootrope en psychopharmacologie. Actual Pharmac. 25e serie: 115-156. Masson,

Paris.

GIURGEA, C. (1973). The “nootropic” approach to the pharmacology of the integrative activity of the brain. Cond. Reflex 8^: 108-115.

GIURGEA, C. (1976). Piracetam: Nootropic pharmacology in neurointegrative activity. In: Current Developmenta in Payohopharmacology, Vol. Ill, pp. 223-273. Spectrum, New York.

GIURGEA, C. (1977). The pharmacology of nootropic drugs: Geropsychiatric implications. In: Neuro-Psychopharmacology, Proc. 10th Congreas of the Collegium Internationale Neuro-Payaho-pharmacologicum, Deniker, P., Radouco-Thomas, C. and Villeneuve, A. (eds.) Pergamon Press, Oxford.

GIURGEA, C., LEFEVRE, D., LESCRENIER, C. and DAVID-REMACLE, M. (1971). Pharmacological protection against hypoxia-induced amnesia in rats. P8ychopharmac. (Berl.) 20: 160-168.

GIURGEA, C. and MOURAVIEFF-LESUISSE, F. (1971). Pharmacological studies on an elimentary model of learning. The fixation of an experience at spinal level: Part I. Pharmacological reactivity of the spinal cord fixation time. Arch. Int. Pharmacodyn. 191: 279-291.

GIURGEA, C. and MOURAVIEFF-LESUISSE, F. (1972). Effet facilitateur du piracetam sur un app-rentissage rdpdtitif chex le rat. J. Pharmac. (Paris). 3: 17-30.

GIURGEA, C., MOURAVIEFF-LESUISSE, F. and LEEMANS, R. (19707. Correlations dlectropharmacolo-giques au cours de l’anoxie oxyprive chez le lapin en respiration libre ou artificielle.

Rev. Neurol. (Paris). 122: 484-486.

GIURGEA, C. and M0YERS00NS, F. (1972). On the pharmacology of cortical evoked potentials. Arch. Int. Pharmacodyn. 199: 67-78.

. Giurgea and H. Salama

246

GIURGEA, C. and MOYERSOONS, F. (1974). Contribution & l’dtude dlectrophysiologique et pharma-cologique de la transmission calleuse. Colloque Internationale’ “Les syndromes de disconnexion calleuse chez l’homme1‘, Lyon.

GIURGEA, C., MOYERSOONS, F. and EVRAERD, A. (1967). A GABA-related hypothesis on the mechanism of action of the anti-motion drugs. Arch. Int. Pharmaaodyn. 166: 238-251.

GOBERT, J.G. (1972). Genfese d’un medicament: le piracetam. MAtabolisation et recherche bio-chimique. J. Pham. Belg. 27: 281-304.

GOURET, C. and RAYNAUD, G. (1976). Utilisation du test de la boite h deux compartiments pour la recherche de substances protAgeant le rat contre 1’amnAsie par hypoxie. IntArfit et limites de la mAthode. J. Pharmao. (Paris). ]_■ 161-175.

GUILMOT, P.H. and VAN EX, R. (1975). Effets du piracetam sur certains symptSmes-cibles de la sdnescence. Ars. Medici. 30: 791-803.

HEINITZ, M. (1975). Neue Aspekte zur Therapie der zerebralen Insufficienz durch Piracetam. Fortschr. Medizin. £3.: 293-298.

ISAKSSON, A., LAGERGREN, K. and WENNBERG, A. (1975). Interaction between heart rate and spectral parameters of the EEG. A pilot study on piracetam-treated patients with cardiac pacemaker. In: Quantitative Analysis of the EEG Methods and Applications. Proc. 2nd Symposium of the Study Group for EEG-Methodology, pp. 149-157, Telefunken-AEG, Constanz, DBR.

ISAKSSON, A., LAGERGREN, K. and WENNBERG, A. (1976). Visible and non visible EEG changes demonstrated by Spectral Parameters Analysis. Electroencephal. Clin. Neurophysiol. 41:

225-226.

LAGERGREN, K. and LEVANDER, S.E. (1974). Effect of piracetam upon performance at varied heart rate. Psychopharwac. (Bert.) 39: 97-104.

MATTHIES, H. and OTT, T. (1975). Differentiation of substances influencing acquisition or retention of learned behavior. Abstr. 6th Intern. Congress of Pharmacology, Helsinki, Finland, p. 370, communication number 878.

MINDUS, P., CRONHOLM, B., LEVANDER, S.E. and SCHALLING, D. (1976). Piracetam-induced improvement of mental performance. Acta Psychiat. Scand. 54j 150-160.

MOYERSOONS, F. and GIURGEA, C. (1974). Protective effect of piracetam in experimental barbiturate intoxication: EEG and behavioral studies. Arch. Int. Pharmaaodyn. 210: 38-48.

MYSLIVICEK, J. (1975). The effect of piracetam on cortical evoked responses in rats stimulated at different post-natal periods. Activ. nerv. sup. (Praha). J_7: 303-304.

MYSLIVICEK, J. and HASSMANOVA, J. (1973). Early sensory deprivation treated with piracetam (ucb-Dipha 6215) in young rats. Activ. nerv. sup. (Praha). J_5: 151-153.

NARIKASHVILI, S.P. (1970). Direct and indirect cortical influences on thalamic nuclei. In: Electrophysiology of the Central Nervous System, Rusinov, V.S. (ed.), pp. 287-298. Plenum Press, New York.

NICHOLSON, V.J. and WOLTHUIS, O.L. (1976a). Effect of the acquisition enhancing drug piracetam on rat cerebral energy metabolism. Comparison with naftidrofuryl and methamphetamine. Bioahem. Pharmac. 25:    2241-2244.

NICHOLSON, V.J. and WOLTHUIS, O.L. (1976b). Differential effects of the acquisition enhancing drug pyrrolidone acetamide (piracetam) on the release of proline from visual and parietal cerebral cortex in vitro. Brain Res. 113: 616-619.

PEDE, J.P., SCHIMPFESSEL, L. and CROKAERT, R. (1971). The action of piracetam on the oxydat-ive phosphorylation. Arch. Int. Physiol. Biochem. 79: 1036-1037.

PLAUCHU, M., NOVEjjJOSSERAND, G., BRESSOT, C. and SOUTEYRAND, P. (1974). Action du Nootropyl (oxo-2-pyrrolidine-acAtamide) dans les Stats confusionnels chez des malades hospitalises dans une service de mAdecine interne. Lyon Mid. 232: 105-108.

ROQUEFEUIL, B., ESCURET, E. and VIGUIE, E. (1975). Etude mAtabolique du piracetam en rAan-imation neuro-chirurgicale. AgresSologie 16:    43-62.

SARA, S. and LEFEVRE, D. (1972). Hypoxia-induced amnesia in one-trial learning and pharmacological protection by piracetam. Psychopharmac. (Berl.).    15:    32-40.

SARA, S.J. and DAVID-REMACLE, M. (1974). Recovery from electroconvulsive shock-induced amnesia by exposure to the training environment. Pharmacological enhancement by piracetam. Psychopharmac. (Berl.) 36: 59-66.

SCHILLER, E. (1974). Elektromikroskopische und mikro-interferometrische Beitrdge zum Wirk-ungsmechanismus von Piracetam. Verh. Dtsch. Ges. Path. _58: 578.

SCHULTE-AM-ESCH, J. and PFEIFER, G. (1974). VorlAufige Erfahrungen mit Piracetam in der Intensivtherapie schwerer Schadel-Him-Verletzungen. Mediz. Klin. 6£: 1235-1238.

SHEPHERD, M. (1972). The classification of psychotropic drugs. Psychol. Med. 2^: 96-110.

‘S J0NGERS, J.J., G0LARD, G., MISERQUE, P., THIEBAULD, C., VOGELAERE, P., POLDERMAN, J. and SEGERS, M. (1975). Effets cardio-circulatoires et neuromusculaires du piracetam, au repos et A l’effort, chez des jeunes sujets bien portants. Brux.-Mid. 55: 117-140.

STEGINK, A.J. (1972). The clinical use of piracetam, a new nootropic drug. Arzneim. Forsch.

(Drug Research) 22: 975-977.

Nootropic drugs

247

VOELKEL, A. (1975). Ueber der Wirkungsprofil von Piracetam bei Psychosyndromen und symptoma-tischen Psychosen. Arzneim. Forsch. (Drug Research) 24: 1127-1129.

WECKROTH, J. (1975). On the effect of piracetam after prolonged use of alcohol on certain performance traits and traits of subjectively rated mental states. Proc. 31st Intern. Congress on Alcoholism and Drug Dependence, Bangkok.

WECKROTH, J. and MIKKONEN, H. (1972). On the effect of ucb 6215 on certain intellectual

perceptual and psychomotor performance traits and traits of subjectively rated mental states. Proc. 30th Intern. Congress on Alcoholism and Drug Dependence, Amsterdam.

WEDL, W. and SUCHENWIRTH, R.M.A. (1977). Eigenwirkungen des GABA-AbkSmmlings Piracetam im doppelten Blindversuch bei gesunden Probanden. Nervenarzt 48: 58-60.

WOLTHUIS, 0. (1971). Experiments with ucb 6215, a drug which enhances acquisition in rats; its effects compared with those of metamphetamine. Eur. J. Pharmac. _^6: 283-297.

1

Partly supported by IRSIA research grant No. 20310.

2

Personal communications.

2.3.1. Animal pharmacology

Table 2 summarizes the main existing data in this field.

3

See also Giurgea, (1976)

2.5.2. Clinical pharmacology

In normal subjects (Oswald and Lewis, 1972, pers. comm.; S. Jongers et at., 1975) no side effects or “doping” effects were ever observed. Nor did piracetam induce any sedation, tran-quillisation, locomotor stimulation or psychodysleptic symptomatology in the more important patient material for whom reliable data are available.

3. Biochemical Considerations

Currently available CNS biochemical data for piracetam are derived mainly from studies mads on the whole brain. It is therefore difficult to discuss them thoroughly in the light of the

telencephalic hypothesis.

We shall however briefly mention here the essential metabolic data and refer the interested reader to more specialised publications (Pede et at., 1971; Gobert 1972).

Three major points are to be emphasized:

(a)    Piracetam in all animal species studied, including man, is practically not metabolised. About 96-98% of the administered substance, by any route of administration, is eliminated mostly in the urine and secondarily in the faeces.

(b)    The plasma half-life of piracetam is about 2.5 hr in the dog and about 4.5 hr in

man.

Leave a Reply